The Chiba Medical Society
School of Medicine, Chiba University
Chiba University Hospital
The Chiba Medical Society HOME Chiba Medical Journal

Chiba Medical Journal
Journal Index
〔Original Paper〕
Gaze recognition in high-functioning autistic patients:
evidence from functional MRI

Hiroko Takebayashi1), Hideo Matsumoto2), Masahiro Ogai1), Yoshimoto Sekine1) Katsuaki Suzuki1), Hideo Matsuzaki1), John Suckling3), Fukujirou Ozawa4) Rinmei Fukuda5), Ichiro Uchiyama6), Kazuhiko Nakamura1), Yoshio Minabe1) Haruo Isoda7), Norio Mori1) and Nori Takei1, 8)
(Received November 29, 2005, Accepted November 30, 2005)

SUMMARY
 We examined whether patients with high-functioning autistic disorder (AD) would exhibit abnormal activation in brain regions implicated in the functioning of theory of mind (TOM) during gaze recognition. We investigated brain activity during gaze recognition in 5 patients with high-functioning AD and 9 normal subjects, using functional magnetic resonance imaging. On the gaze task, more activation was found in the left middle frontal gyrus, the right intraparietal sulcus, and the precentral and inferior parietal gyri bilaterally in controls than in AD patients, whereas the patient group showed more powerful signal changes in the left superior temporal gyrus, the right insula, and the right medial frontal gyrus. These results suggest that high-functioning AD patients have functional abnormalities not only in TOM-related brain regions, but also in widely distributed brain regions that are not normally activated upon the processing of information from another person’s gaze.

Key Words
high-functioning autism, gaze recognition; functional MRI, theory of mind

 
Ⅰ.Introduction
Autistic disorder (AD) is characterized by a dysfunction of communication and impaired development with respect to the acquisition of social skills. Some authors have suggested that AD is a condition involving an impaired “Theory of Mind” (TOM) or mentalizing (Baron-Cohen et al[1]; Happe and Frith[2]; Castelli et al[3]). TOM refers to the ability to perceive the mental states of other persons, such as the beliefs and intentions of others; that is, with this function, one can understand and predict the behavior of other people by reasoning about their mental states (Premack and Woodruff[4]).

An abundance of information derived from the expressions on the human face, and face-to-face communication is vital in interpersonal interactions. Using neuroimaging technology, some groups have focused on human faces as stimuli to investigate TOM function in AD patients. Three main approaches have been applied, i.e., face recognition, facial expression, and eye gaze experiments. For example, as regards the face recognition, Schultz et al[5]]have revealed using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) that AD patients exhibit greater activation in the inferior temporal gyri on a face discrimination task than do control subjects. Since the activated areas were generally involved in the completion of non-face object perception tasks, their results suggested that AD patients may process human faces merely as objects. A study reported by Critchley et al[6]demonstrated widespread functional abnormalities in the limbic region, the cerebellum, and the extrastriate visual cortices during the explicit and implicit processing of emotional facial expressions in individuals with AD. In our previous fMRI study, we observed differential brain function between patients with AD and normal controls during tasks involving the recognition of facially expressed emotions (Ogai et al[7]). Baron-Cohen et al[8], who studied brain function in autistic patients and normal subjects using a task involving the recognition of a mental state expressed in another person’s eyes, reported less activation in the amygdala and more activation in the superior temporal gyrus in the AD patients than in the normal subjects.

Baron-Cohen[9]has also suggested that the interpretation of another person’s gaze plays an important role in a normally functioning TOM. In accord with this suggestion, some functional imaging studies of normal volunteers have shown that tasks involving the TOM, as well as tasks involving eye gaze processing, evoked activation in similar brain regions, such as the posterior superior temporal sulcus (Hoffman and Haxby[10]), the medial frontal areas (Calder et al[11]), and the amygdala (Kawashima et al[12]). Therefore, the presentation of gaze discriminating tasks as a social stimulus can be considered to be sufficient to activate the mechanisms by which individuals process and interpret the mental states of others. We tested the hypothesis that on an eye-gaze paradigm in the framework of fMRI, AD patients with a relatively high IQ (i.e., individuals with high-functioning AD) would show abnormal activation in brain regions implicated in the functioning of TOM.

Ⅱ.Materials and Methods
1.Subjects
We studied 5 right-handed patients satisfying the DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association[13])criteria for AD and 9 right-handed normal subjects who were matched with the patients in terms of age, IQ (both groups had an IQ of above 85 on the full Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Revision, WAIS-R), educational level, and socioeconomic status (Table 1). Diagnosis of the patient group was made in an operationalized manner identical to that of the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R; Lord et al[14]). The patient group consisted of individuals with high-functioning AD. No comorbidity of psychiatric problems other than AD was identified in any of the subjects. None of the patients were medicated at the time of the study. After providing the subjects with a complete description of the study, written informed consent was obtained from all participants before entry into the study, in compliance with the procedures of the Ethics Committee of the Hamamatsu University School of Medicine.

Fig. 1 Stimuli for the gaze task trial. Participants were presented with pictures of either an “averted” (a) or a “direct” (b) gaze.
2.Experimental paradigms and procedure
As the stimulus during the scanning, we implemented a gaze recognition task. During the task, pictures of other people’s eyes, showing both direct and averted gazes, were presented (Fig. 1). These eye gaze stimuli were counterbalanced for age and gender. The same gaze orientations were used for all stimuli. The person’s head tilted 15° leftward from the front for all the conditions. In the direct condition, the person’s gaze was directed toward the camera. In the averted condition, the person’s gaze was directed 15° leftwards from the front. The subjects were instructed to determine the direction of the gaze. Stimuli were presented through goggles worn by the subjects. Before the MRI scan, the subjects were presented with an example that was not used during the actual scanning time; this example appeared on the computer monitor, and was used to familiarize the subjects with the experimental procedure and the experimental conditions, in order to minimize anxiety and enhance task performance. After this familiarization, the subjects lay supine on the scanning table and were fitted with goggles.

We used an alternating on/off box-car design of 30 sec duration for each on or off condition, and there was a total of 5 cycles of “on/off” alternation. Each stimulus was presented for 3.5 sec with an interval of 0.25 sec. In the “on” condition, 8 different faces with a direct gaze were presented to the subjects. In the “off” condition, the same 8 faces were shown with an averted gaze. During the scanning procedure, subjects were asked to concentrate on their judgment of the direction of the gaze. After the scanning session, the subjects immediately performed a test, on the perception of the gaze presented in the experiments.

The brain activation reported in this article refers to periodic BOLD (blood oxygenation level-dependent; Ogawa et al[15]) signal changes at the fundamental experimental frequency.

3.Image acquisition
One hundred T2-weighted gradient-echo (TR=3000, TE=40 ms) echoplanar MR images depicting BOLD contrast were acquired using a 1.5 Tesla scanner GE Signa system (General Electric, Milwaukee, Wisconsin, USA) for a 5-min period at 14 near-axial non-contiguous 7-mm thick planes (0.7 mm gap) parallel to the intercommissural (AC-PC) line encompassing the whole brain. During the same session, a 43-slice, high-resolution inversion recovery echo planar image of the whole brain was also acquired, again parallel to the AC-PC line, from each subject (TI=180 ms, TE=73 ms, TR=16 s, 1.5 mm plane resolution, 3 mm slice thickness, 8 signal averages) in order to determine the subsequent anatomical registration.

4.Data analysis
Data were processed by the Brain Activation and Morphological Mapping software (University of Cambridge, UK: http://www-bmu.psychiatry.cam.ac.uk/software/). Following correction for the effects of subject’s motion during image acquisition (Bullmore et al[16]), linear regression was used to estimate experimentally induced signal changes at each voxel between the direct (“on”) and averted eye gaze (“off”) conditions. Prior to model fitting, this single contrast design matrix was convolved with a pair of Poisson kernels to model locally variable hemodynamics. A test statistic of the standardized power of the response (F*) was computed from the ratio of the sum of the squares due to the model fit and the residual sum of the squares.

Statistic maps representing the standardized effect for each individual at all sessions were registered in the standard space of Talairach and Tournoux[17]by an affine transformation to a template image (Brammer et al[18]).

Differences in the responses (F*) between AD subjects and normal subjects were calculated at each voxel with a second-level regression of the general linear model. These differences were tested for statistical significance by a two-tailed test against a distribution sampled under a null-hypothesis of no group effect by the repeated permutation of group membership of the subjects and by recalculation of the differences. Due to the large number of voxel locations which were tested (typically~300,000), the estimated number of type I errors at reasonable statistical thresholds (P-values) was unacceptably large. Spatial information (clusters) was included in the test statistics in order to reduce the overall number of tests, in addition to improving sensitivity to large areas of low-contrast differences. In brief, the cluster-level analysis involved applying a preliminary probability threshold (P<0.05) to the observed difference maps and setting all subthreshold voxels to zero, thus creating a set of suprathreshold voxel clusters that were spatially contiguous in 3D. An identical procedure was applied to the maps of the simulated null-hypothesis, with the resulting clusters sampling the null-distribution for the cluster statistic. The family wise-error rate was controlled by rejecting the null-hypothesis with a statistical threshold at which only 1 false positive (type I error) cluster per image was expected by chance (for further details, see Bullmore et al[19]).

For behavioral performance, we compared the correct answers in 8 different persons’ eye stimuli of direct and averted gazes, which were used during the actual scanning time, between AD and control groups using t test.

Ⅲ.Results
1.Behavioral performance
There was no significant difference between the autistic and control groups as regards the proportion of accuracy when the subjects were identifying either of the two types of gaze; rates of 67.5% vs. 68.1% were obtained, respectively, for direct gaze (t=-0.03, P=.98), and 97.5% vs. 100% for averted gaze (t=-1.39, P=.19), respectively.

2.fMRI findings
More activation was observed in the controls than in the AD patients in the left middle frontal gyrus, the bilateral precentral gyrus, and the right intraparietal sulcus, as well as in the bilateral inferior parietal lobe (Fig. 2A and Table 2). In contrast, more activation was observed in the patients with AD in the left superior temporal gyrus, the right insula, and the right medial frontal gyrus, as compared to the controls (Fig. 2B).

Fig. 2 (A) Yellow regions denote areas in which more activation was observed in normal controls than in high-functioning autistic patients. (B) Purple regions denote areas in which more activation was observed in high-functioning autistic patients than in normal controls. The number at the upper left of each axial section denotes the axial position relative to the intercommisural line (in millimeters) (i.e., the z coordinate). The right side of each slice represents the left side of the brain. A vertical line of the red cross indicates x coordinate of 0 and a horizontal line indicates y coordinate of 0 in the standard space of Talairach & Tournoux (1988). Table 2. Main brain regions differentially activated by eye gaze task in normal controls and high-functioning autistic patients1

Ⅳ.Discussion
Our results demonstrated that the medial frontal gyrus and the superior temporal gyrus were more activated in AD patients on an eye gaze task than in the control subjects. These brain regions are consistent with the areas implicated in the processing of mental states of other people, i.e., those areas involved in TOM (Fletcher et al[20]; Happe et al[21]; Gallagher et al[22]; Stuss et al[23]; Calarge et al[24]). As mentioned above, the process of reasoning about the significance of another person’s gaze has been suggested to involve a mechanism of TOM (Baron-Cohen[9]). Neuroimaging studies have shown in healthy subjects that tasks directly involving the TOM mechanism as well as eye gaze processing tasks induce activity in these two brain regions (Hoffman and Haxby[10]; Calder et al[11]). Therefore, our findings suggest that processing the gaze of others provokes a dysfunction in TOM-related brain regions in high-functioning AD patients.

Baron-Cohen et al[8]conducted an fMRI study which focused on the mental state expressed in another person’s eyes, and have found that AD patients show less activation in the left amygdala than normal subjects. However, we failed to detect any difference in activation in the amygdala between the AD and control groups. The failure to demonstrate reduced activation in the amygdala (Baron-Cohen et al[8])in our sample of AD patients could have been due to differences in the tasks employed between the two studies. Our task focused on the judgment of the direction of the other person’s gaze, whereas the task used in the study by Baron-Cohen et al[8]incorporated a determination of the complex attributes of emotions expressed in the eyes of others. Although it has been suggested in a previous fMRI study that emotional responses are evoked during the performance of eye gaze tasks in normal volunteers (Kawashima et al[12]), our study did not specifically ask the subjects to identify the mental state expressed in the other person’s eyes. This difference may account for the lack of a functional impairment in the amygdala in our sample of AD patients.

We also found different patterns of activation between AD patients and the control subjects in other brain regions such as the insula, the precentral gyrus, the middle frontal gyrus, and the intraparietal sulcus; these four regions have not been previously indicated to be related to the functioning of TOM, and therefore these findings were not anticipated. Our results revealed that AD patients showed more activation in the insula compared to the control subjects, whereas there was less activation in the precentral gyrus, the middle frontal gyrus, and the intraparietal sulcus in the AD patient group than in the control group. It has been suggested that the insula becomes involved when stimuli directly evoke strong emotional responses, such as disgust (Adolphs et al[25]; Morris et al[26]; Phillips et al[27]). The precentral gyrus has been shown to be associated with visual search processing (Darren et al[28]; Gitelman et al[29]), and patients with AD have been reported to engage more heavily in visual searching on face perception tasks, whereas healthy controls tend to utilize more holistic processing (Hubl et al[30]). The region of the left middle frontal gyrus has been indicated to be related to central executive function (Frith et al[31]; D’Esposito et al[32]; Salmon et al[33]). In addition, Hoffman and Haxby[10]have reported that the intraparietal sulcus is related to eye-gaze perception in normal subjects, which is associated with spatial perception and spatially directed attention (Haxby et al[34]; Corbetta et al[35]; Nobre et al[36]; Corbetta[37]). Our AD patients may have undergone inappropriate emotional responses during the processing of the task used in this study, and, due to abnormalities in specific brain regions, they may have used strategies that differed from those of healthy subjects in order to perform the required task. Alternatively, the affective salience of the images may be relevant to the activation pattern seen in our autistic subjects. Insula activation is associated with processing of interoception, including somatosensory feedback associated with autonomic arousal (Davidson and Irwin[38]; Critchley et al[39]). One possible interpretation of the higher activation pattern of two TOM-related brain regions and the insula in the autistic subjects is that they may have been more affectively aroused by the images than the normal controls, such that feedback modulation of the gaze perception areas such as the superior temporal gyrus occurred. However, widely distributed impairment in the neural circuitry, including in the insula and the other three brain regions, may exist and bear the pathophysiology of autism. It is possible that such widely distributed abnormalities in brain function in AD patients may be reflective of a compensatory process.

Our results suggest that processing the direction of another person’s eyes involves an element of perceiving mental states in others, and such a stimulus appears to be sufficient to disclose functional impairment in TOM-associated brain regions in high-functioning AD patients. Furthermore, our findings also indicate that high-functioning AD patients may, in a compensatory manner, recruit widely distributed brain regions that are not normally required to perform a socially-linked information processing task.

Acknowledgment
We are grateful to the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology, Japan for financial support: Dr. Takei was the recipient of a Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research (project no. 11470201). This study was also supported by a Grant-in-Aid for the Center of Excellence (COE) from the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology, Japan.

要旨
 我々は,高機能自閉症患者が視線弁別の際に「心の理論theory of mind (ToM)」に関連する脳部位の異常活性を認めるかについて検証した。磁気共鳴機能画像法functional Magnetic Resonance Imagingを用いて,5人の高機能自閉症患者と9人の健常者に視線弁別のタスクを行なった。この結果,健常者では患者群に比べて,左中前頭回,右頭頂間溝,両側中心前回,両側下頭頂回により高い活性が認められた。一方,患者群では健常者に比べて左上側頭回,右島,右内側前頭葉により高活性が認められた。以上より,高機能自閉症患者では,(1) ToM関連の脳部位における機能的以上,および (2) 通常,他者の視線からの情報を処理する際には見られない,脳の広範囲にわたる脳部位における機能的異常があることが示唆された。

References
1) Baron-Cohen S, Leslie AM, Frith U. Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition 1985; 21: 37-46. [PubMed]
2) Happe F, Frith U. The neuropsychology of autism. Brain 1996; 119: 1377-400. [PubMed]
3) Castelli F, Frith C, Happe F, Frith U. Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. Brain 2002; 125: 1839-49. [PubMed]
4) Premack D, Woodruff G. Dose the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behav Brain Sci 1978; 1: 515-26.
5) Schultz RT, Gauthier I, Klin A, Fulbright RK, Anderson AW, Volkmar F, Skudlarski P, Lacadie C, Cohen DJ, Gore JC. Abnormal ventral temporal cortical activity during face discrimination among individuals with autism and Asperger syndrome. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2000; 57: 331-40. [PubMed]
6) Critchley HD, Daly EM, Bullmore ET, Williams SC, Van Amelsvoort T, Robertson DM, Rowe A, Phillips M, McAlonan G, Howlin P, Murphy DG. The functional neuroanatomy of social behaviour: changes in cerebral blood flow when people with autistic disorder process facial expressions. Brain 2000; 123: 2203-12. [PubMed]
7) Ogai M, Matsumoto H, Suzuki K, Ozawa F, Fukuda R, Uchiyama I, Suckling J, Isoda H, Mori N, Takei N. fMRI study of recognition of facial expressions in high-functioning autistic patients. Neuroreport 2003; 14: 559-63. [PubMed]
8) Baron-Cohen S, Ring HA, Wheelwright S, Bullmore ET, Brammer MJ, Simmons A, Williams SC. Social intelligence in the normal and autistic brain: an fMRI study. Eur J Neurosci 1999; 11: 1891-8. [Full text]
9) Baron-Cohen S. Mindblindness: an essay on autism and theory of mind, Cambridge MA. MIT Press, 1997. [PubMed]
10) Hoffman EA, Haxby JV. Distinct representations of eye gaze and identity in the distributed human neural system for face perception. Nat Neurosci 2000; 3: 80-4.[PubMed]
11) Calder AJ, Lawrence AD, Keane J, Scott SK, Owen AM, Christoffels I, Young AW. Reading the mind from eye gaze. Neuropsychologia 2002; 40: 1129-38.[ELSEVIER Full text]
12) Kawashima R, Sugiura M, Kato T, Nakamura A, Hatano K, Ito K, Fukuda H, Kojima S, Nakamura K. The human amygdala plays an important role in gaze monitoring. A PET study. Brain 1999; 122: 779-83.[PubMed],[Full text]
13) American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition; DSM-IV, Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press Incorporated, 1994.[PubMed]
14) Lord C, Rutter M, Le Couteur A. Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 1994; 24: 659-85.[PubMed]
15) Ogawa S, Lee TM, Kay AR, Tank DW. Brain magnetic resonance imaging with contrast dependent on blood oxygenation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1990; 87: 9868-72.[Full text]
16) Bullmore ET, Brammer MJ, Rabe-Hesketh S, Curtis VA, Morris RG, Williams SCR, Sharma T, McGuire PK. Methods for diagnosis and treatment of stimulus-correlated motion in generic brain activation studies using fMRI. Hum Brain Mapp 1999; 7: 38-48.[PubMed]
17) Talairach J, Tournoux P. Co-Plannar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain, New York: Thieme, 1988.[PubMed]
18) Brammer MJ, Bullmore ET, Simmons A, Williams SC, Grasby PM, Howard RJ, Woodruff PW, Rabe-Hesketh S. Generic brain activation mapping in functional magnetic resonance imaging: a nonparametric approach. Magn Reson Imaging 1997; 15: 763-70.[Full text]
19) Bullmore ET, Suckling J, Overmeyer S, Rabe-Hesketh S, Taylor E, Brammer MJ. Global, voxel and cluster tests, by theory and permutation for a difference between two groups of structural MR images of the brain. IEEE Trans Med Imaging 1999; 18: 32-42.[PubMed]
20) Fletcher PC, Happe F, Frith U, Baker SC, Dolan RJ, Frackowiak RS, Frith CD. Other minds in the brain: a functional imaging study of “theory of mind” in story comprehension. Cognition 1995; 57: 109-28. [Full text]
21) Happe F, Ehlers S, Fletcher P, Frith U, Johansson M, Gillberg C, Dolan R, Frackowiak R, Frith C. ‘Theory of mind’ in the brain. Evidence from a PET scan study of Asperger syndrome. Neuroreport 1996; 20: 197-201.[PubMed]
22) Gallagher HL, Happe F, Brunswick N, Fletcher PC, Frith U, Frith CD. Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fMRI study of ‘theory of mind’ in verbal and nonverbal tasks. Neuropsychologia 2000; 38: 11-21.[Full text]
23) Stuss DT, Gallup GGJ, Alexander MP. The frontal lobes are necessary for ‘theory of mind’. Brain 2001; 124: 279-86.[PubMed] [Full text]
24) Calarge C, Andreasen NC, O’Leary DS. Visualizing how one brain understands another: a PET study of theory of mind. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160: 1954-64.[Full text]
25) Adolphs R, Tranel D, Damasio H, Damasio A. Impaired recognition of emotion in facial expressions following bilateral damage to the human amygdala. Nature 1994; 15: 669-72.[PubMed]
26) Morris JS, Frith CD, Perrett DI, Rowland D, Young AW, Calder AJ, Dolan RJ. A differential neural response in the human amygdala to fearful and happy facial expressions. Nature 1996; 31: 812-5.[PubMed]
27) Phillips ML, Young AW, Senior C, Brammer M, Andrew C, Calder AJ, Bullmore ET, Perrett DI, Rowland D, Williams SC, Gray JA, David AS. A specific neural substrate for perceiving facial expressions of disgust. Nature 1997; 2: 495-8.[PubMed]
28) Darren RG, Todd BP, Kevin SL, M-Marsel, Real-Time Monitoring of Eye Movements Using Infrared Video-oculography during Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging of the Frontal Eye Fields. NeuroImage 2000; 11: 58-65.[PubMed], [Full text]
29) Gitelman DR, Parrish TB, Friston KJ, Mesulam MM. Functional anatomy of visual search: regional segregations within the frontal eye fields and effective connectivity of the superior colliculus. NeuroImage 2002; 15: 970-82.[Full text]
30) Hubl D, Bolte S, Feineis-Matthews S, Lanfermann H, Federspiel A, Strik W, Poustka F, Dierks T. Functional imbalance of visual pathways indicates alternative face processing strategies in autism. Neurology 2003; 61: 1232-7.[PubMed]
31) Frith CD, Friston KJ, Liddle PF, Frackowiak RS. A PET study of word finding. Neuropsychologia 1991; 29: 1137-48.[PubMed]
32) D’Esposito M, Detre JA, Alsop DC, Shin RK, Atlas S, Grossman M. The neural basis of the central executive system of working memory. Nature 1995; 378: 279-81.[PubMed]
33) Salmon E, Van der Linden M, Collette F, Delfiore G, Maquet P, Degueldre C, Luxen A, Franck G. Regional brain activity during working memory tasks. Brain 1996; 119: 1617-25.[PubMed]
34) Haxby JV, Horwitz B, Ungerleider LG, Maisog JM, Pietrini P, Grady CL. The functional organization of human extrastriate cortex: a PET-rCBF study of selective attention to faces and locations. J Neurosci 1994; 14: 6336-53.[PubMed]
35) Corbetta M, Shulman GL, Miezin FM, Petersen SE. Superior parietal cortex activation during spatial attention shifts and visual feature conjunction. Science 1995; 270: 802-5.[PubMed]
36) Nobre AC, Sebestyen G.N, Gitelman DR, Mesulam MM, Frackowiak RS, Frith, C.D. Functional localization of the system for visuospatial attention using positron emission tomography. Brain 1997; 120: 515-33.[Full text]
37) Corbetta M. Frontoparietal cortical networks for directing attention and the eye to visual locations: identical, independent, or overlapping neural systems? Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1998; 95: 831-8.[Full text],[Full text]
38) Davidson RJ, Irwin W. The functional neuroanatomy of emotion and affective style. Trends Cogn Sci 1999; 3: 11-21.[PubMed],[Full text]
39) Critchley HD, Melmed RN, Featherstone E, Mathias CJ, Dolan RJ. Volitional control of autonomic arousal: a functional magnetic resonance study. NeuroImage 2002; 16: 909-19.[PubMed],[Full text]


Others
1) Department of Psychiatry and Neurology, Hamamatsu University School of Medicine, Hamamatsu 431-3192.
2) Department of Psychiatry & Behavioral Science, Tokai University School of Medicine, Isehara 259-1193.
3) Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Addenbrooke’s Hospital, Cambridge CB2 2QQ, UK.
4) The 5th Research Laboratory Hamamatsu Photonics K.K., 5000, Hirakuchi, Hamakita 434-0041.
5) Department of Psychiatry, Tokyo University, Tokyo 113-8655.
6) Department of Psychology, Doshisha University, Kyoto 602-8580.
7) Department of Radiology, Hamamatsu University School of Medicine, Hamamatsu 431-3192.
8) Department of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry, London SE5 8AF, UK.
竹林啓子1),松本英夫2),小粥正博1),関根吉統1),鈴木勝昭1),松崎英夫1),John Suckling3),小澤福示郎4),福田倫明5),内山伊知郎6),中村和彦1),三辺義雄1),礒田治夫7),森 則夫1),武井教使1, 8): 高機能自閉症患者における視線弁別に関する機能的MRI研究.
1) 浜松医科大学精神神経科,2) 東海大学医学部精神科,3) ケンブリッジ大学精神科Addenbrooke病院
4) 浜松ホトニクス中央研究所第5研究室,5) 東京大学医学部精神神経科,6) 同志社大学文学部心理学科
7) 浜松医科大学放射線科,8) ロンドン大学精神医学研究所
Tel. 053-435-2295. Fax. 053-435-3621. e-mail: ntakei@hama-med.ac.jp
2005年11月29日受付,2005年11月30日受理.
.
Copyright (C) 2002 The Chiba Medical Society. All Rights Reserved.